CHARLES Keil's paper Motion and Feeling through Music (1966) was a response to Leonard B. Meyer's influential book Emotion and Meaning in Music (1956), in which Meyer argued that the expressivity of music and its meaning are rooted in musical syntax. In his paper, Keil acknowledged that syntax may be important for the expressive content of notated compositions from the Western art music repertoire (so-called classical music). Yet, he claimed that syntax does not explain the expressivity of many other forms of music performance around the world. With this, he referred to music that includes substantial proportions of improvisation and spontaneity, music that is understood as a process, not as a work, music that is propelled by a vital drive (a mysterious essentialist concept borrowed from André Hodeir's 1956 classic jazz analysis), and music that has groove, i.e., music that motivates motor responses in listeners.

Keil (1966) hypothesised that vital drive does not result from musical syntax, but from the way music is performed. He exemplified this with reference to jazz, stating that a drummer's tap (i.e., the bodily process of producing sounds on drums or cymbals) is essential to the drive of the music. He distinguished between two types of drummers: 'on-top' drummers hit the cymbals exactly on the metronomic beat by keeping "the stick close to the cymbal, arm fairly stationary with the stroke moving perpendicular to the cymbal" (Keil, 1966, p. 342). Conversely, 'laid-back' drummers delay accents on some notes and appear to attack the cymbal more "horizontally" (Keil, 1966, p. 342). Both types of drummers, Keil asserted, are capable of generating great vital drive, but they do it differently, and the effect on the listeners will be different, too. Vital drive in jazz drumming thus depends on the concrete physical interaction between a drummer's body and their instrument.

Keil then added the bassists to the mix and categorised them into 'stringy' bassists, on one hand, who play high on the strings producing light yet sustained melodious notes. He contrasted them with 'chunky' bassists, on the other hand, who pluck the strings near the bridge and have a shorter, heavier, percussive tone. According to Keil, 'on-top' drummers pair well with 'chunky' bassists. These pairs lay down a rock solid fundament that won't be shaken by a strong countercurrent of syncopations played by soloists such as the idiosyncratic Thelonious Monk. 'Laid back' drummers and 'stringy' bassists also make a good combination: according to Keil, they can swing on their own, and allow a melodist solo player such as Miles Davis to drop behind the beat without affecting the vital drive of the entire performance. Keil agreed that other combinations do exist and may be successful, but he observed that stringy/on-top teams may have a tendency to rush the tempo, whereas chunky/laid-back pairs may generate a "rather sluggish vital drive" (Keil, 1966, p. 344).

Keil's core point was that the expressivity of music arises in the physical, collaborative act of performing, and that the "how" of this process is at least as important as the "what" that is being performed. This point was difficult to prove in the 1960s. One important reason for this lack of impact was methodological: In 1966, there was no way to demonstrate scientifically that these traces of the performance process or "participatory discrepancies" (PDs; see Keil, 1987) even existed. This situation started to change in the 1980s and 1990s, when researchers such as Bengtsson and Gabrielsson (1983), Rose (1989), Repp (1992), Alén (1995), and Prögler (1995) found and applied methods to measure the exact onset times of musical notes. The timing measurements revealed a plethora of microscopic life in shortest time spans of performed music, and they showed that participatory discrepancies indeed exist in the time domain of music (much to the relief of Keil himself; Keil, 1995, p. 2). This triggered a series of microtiming analyses that studied performances from various musical contexts and genres, be it Western art music (Dodson, 2011; Goebl et al., 2004; Repp, 1992, 1997, 1998; Senn et al., 2009, 2012), jazz (Datseris et al., 2019; Ellis, 1991; Friberg & Sundström, 1997, 2002; Honing & Haas, 2008; Nelias et al., 2022), samba or other forms of popular music (Haugen, 2014, Hosken et al., 2021; Naveda et al., 2011).

Keil not only discussed the mere existence of PDs, but also their effect on listeners, claiming that "the power of music is in its participatory discrepancies […]. Music, to be personally involving and socially valuable, must be 'out of time' and 'out of tune'" (Keil, 1987, p. 275). This motivated research to investigate

whether microtiming deviations were relevant to triggering the groove experience, understood as an inner urge to move in response to the music (a form of personal involvement in the music). However, in listening experiments, groove ratings of stimuli with patterned microtiming either did not exceed those of perfectly quantized stimuli (Senn et al., 2016), obtained significantly lower ratings (Davies et al., 2013; Frühauf et al., 2013; Skaansar et al., 2019), or were irrelevant (Madison et al., 2011). This indicates that the music does not necessarily have to be 'out of time' to be personally involving.

Was Keil wrong? Or was his concept of participatory discrepancies misinterpreted by the groove research community? The work by Câmara, Danielsen, and the Oslo RITMO center points at a possible third option, namely that the groove experiments did not go far enough in their reproduction of PDs. As of today, the PD manipulations consisted exclusively in displacing events in time, without changing the sounds themselves. However, Keil had suggested that on-top and laid-back drummers not only struck the drums and cymbals at a different time relative to the beat, but that they also used different playing techniques. The different techniques produce notes that vary in terms of timbre, loudness, and articulation. In their recent work, Câmara, Danielsen, and the Oslo RITMO team have shown that these variations are as real in actual performance as the temporal displacements. Bassists tend to play louder when they play ahead of the beat (Câmara et al., 2020a), compared to on or after the beat. Guitarists use darker timbres, longer notes, slower movements and elongated attacks when they play laid-back, compared to other playing styles (Câmara et al., 2020a; Câmara et al., 2023). Drummers play the back-beat louder if they articulate it in a laid-back feel (Câmara et al., 2020b). Such changes of dynamics, timbre and movement style in the production of sounds also affect the perception of timing in listeners (Danielsen et al., 2019). All this resonates with Keil's own description of PDs as having many different dimensions (Keil, 1995, p. 7).

Combined, Câmara and colleagues' results suggest that the operationalisation of PDs as temporal displacements of identical sounds (as found in microtiming-related groove research such as Senn et al., 2016) might be overly simplistic. The different feels affect musicians' sound production in a variety of dimensions, leave their traces in the performed music, and might be relevant to the effect of the music on the listeners. In their studies, Câmara and colleagues provide important groundwork for a re-evaluation of Keil's claim that the power of music lies in its participatory discrepancies. Keil acknowledged that his idea "didn't trigger an intellectual revolution or shift the paradigm in musicology" (Keil, 1995, p. 2) back in the 1960s and 1970s, and it still seems to be fairly unimportant for musicology today, more than half a decade after the publication of Motion and Feeling through Music. Yet, Keil's revolution may just be very, very slow in the making.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This article was copyedited by Tanushree Agrawal and layout edited by Jonathan Tang.

NOTES

  1. Correspondence can be addressed to: Olivier Senn, Lucerne University of Applied Sciences and Arts, Arsenalstrasse 28a, 6010 Kriens, Switzerland, olivier.senn@hslu.ch.
    Return to Text

REFERENCES

  • Alén, O. (1995). Rhythm as duration of sounds in Tumba Francesa. Ethnomusicology, 39(1), 55–71. https://doi.org/10.2307/852200
  • Bengtsson, I., & Gabrielsson, A. (1983). Analysis and synthesis of musical rhythm. In Studies of music performance (pp. 27–60). Royal Swedish Academy of Music.
  • Câmara, G. S., Nymoen, K., Lartillot, O., & Danielsen, A. (2020a). Effects of instructed timing on electric guitar and bass sound in groove performance. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 147(2), 1028–1041. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0000724
  • Câmara, G. S., Nymoen, K., Lartillot, O., & Danielsen, A. (2020b). Timing Is Everything…Or Is It? Effects of Instructed Timing Style, Reference, and Pattern on Drum Kit Sound in Groove-Based Performance. Music Perception, 38(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2020.38.1.1
  • Câmara, G. S., Sioros, G., Nymoen, K., Haugen, M. R., Danielsen, A. (2023). Sound-producing actions in guitar performance of groove-based microrhythm. Empirical Musicology Review, 18(1), 21–36. https://doi.org/10.18061/emr.v18i1.9124
  • Danielsen, A., Nymoen, K., Anderson, E., Câmara, G. S., Langerød, M. T., Thompson, M. R., & London, J. (2019). Where is the beat in that note? Effects of attack, duration, and frequency on the perceived timing of musical and quasi-musical sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45, 402–418. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000611
  • Datseris, G., Ziereis, A., Albrecht, T., Hagmayer, Y., Priesemann, V., & Geisel, T. (2019). Microtiming Deviations and Swing Feel in Jazz. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 19824. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-55981-3
  • Davies, M., Madison, G., Silva, P., & Gouyon, F. (2013). The Effect of Microtiming Deviations on the Perception of Groove in Short Rhythms. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30(5), 497–510. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2013.30.5.497
  • Dodson, A. (2011). Expressive Asynchrony in a Recording of Chopin's Prelude No. 6 in B Minor by Vladimir de Pachmann. Music Theory Spectrum, 33(1), 59–64. https://doi.org/10.1525/mts.2011.33.1.59
  • Ellis, M. (1991). An analysis of swing subdivision and asynchronization in three jazz saxophonists. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 73(3), 707–713. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1991.73.3.707
  • Friberg, A., & Sundström, A. (1997). Preferred swing ratio in jazz as a function of tempo. Speech, Music & Hearing, 4, 19–28.
  • Friberg, A., & Sundström, A. (2002). Swing ratios and ensemble timing in jazz performance: Evidence for a common rhythmic pattern. Music Perception, 19(3), 333–349. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2002.19.3.333
  • Frühauf, J., Kopiez, R., & Platz, F. (2013). Music on the timing grid: The influence of microtiming on the perceived groove quality of a simple drum pattern performance. Musicae Scientiae, 17(2), 246–260. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864913486793
  • Goebl, W., Pampalk, E., & Widmer, G. (2004). Exploring expressive performance trajectories: Six famous pianists play six Chopin pieces. Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition.
  • Haugen, M. R. (2014). Rhythmical structures in music and body motion in Afro-Brazilian samba and Norwegian telespringar. International Conference of Students of Systematic Musicology, 0(0). https://journals.gold.ac.uk/index.php/sysmus14/article/view/233
  • Hodeir, A. (1956). Jazz: Its evolution and essence.
  • Honing, H., & Haas, W. B. de. (2008). Swing once more: Relating timing and tempo in expert Jazz drumming. Music Perception, 25(5), 471–476. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2008.25.5.471
  • Hosken, F., Bechtold, T., Hoesl, F., Kilchenmann, L., & Senn, O. (2021). Drum Groove Corpora. Empirical Musicology Review, 16(1), 114-123. https://doi.org/10.18061/emr.v16i1.7642
  • Keil, C. (1966). Motion and feeling through music. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 24(3), 337–349.
  • Keil, C. (1987). Participatory discrepancies and the power of music. Cultural Anthropology, 2(3), 275–283. https://doi.org/10.1525/can.1987.2.3.02a00010
  • Keil, C. (1995). The theory of participatory discrepancies: A progress report. Ethnomusicology, 39(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.2307/852198
  • Madison, G., Gouyon, F., Ullén, F., & Hörnström, K. (2011). Modeling the tendency for music to induce movement in humans: First correlations with low-level audio descriptors across music genres. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1578–1594. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024323
  • Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. University of Chicago Press.
  • Naveda, L., Gouyon, F., Guedes, C., & Leman, M. (2011). Microtiming Patterns and Interactions with Musical Properties in Samba Music. Journal of New Music Research, 40(3), 225–238. https://doi.org/10.1080/09298215.2011.603833
  • Nelias, C., Sturm, E. M., Albrecht, T., Hagmayer, Y., & Geisel, T. (2022). Downbeat delays are a key component of swing in jazz. Communications Physics, 5(1), 237. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42005-022-00995-z
  • Prögler, J. A. (1995). Searching for swing: Participatory discrepancies in the jazz rhythm section. Ethnomusicology, 39(1), 21–54. https://doi.org/10.2307/852199
  • Repp, B. H. (1992). Diversity and commonality in music performance: An analysis of timing microstructure in Schumann's Träumerei. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 92(5), 2546–2568. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.404425
  • Repp, B. H. (1997). Expressive timing in a Debussy Prelude: A comparison of student and expert pianists. Musicae Scientiae, 1(2), 257–268. https://doi.org/10.1177/102986499700100206
  • Repp, B. H. (1998). Obligatory "expectations" of expressive timing induced by perception of musical structure. Psychological Research, 61(1), 33–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004260050011
  • Rose, R. F. (1989). An analysis of timing in jazz rhythm section performance [PhD Thesis]. University of Texas.
  • Senn, O., Camp, M.-A., & Kilchenmann, L. (2009). Expressive timing: Martha Argerich plays Chopin's Prelude op. 28/4 in E minor. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Performance Science 2009, 107–112.
  • Senn, O., Kilchenmann, L., & Camp, M.-A. (2012). A turbulent acceleration into the stretto: Martha Argerich plays Chopin's Prelude op. 28/4 in E minor. Dissonance, 120, 31–35.
  • Senn, O., Kilchenmann, L., von Georgi, R., & Bullerjahn, C. (2016). The Effect of Expert Performance Microtiming on Listeners' Experience of Groove in Swing or Funk Music. Frontiers in Psychology: Performance Science, 7(1487), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01487
  • Skaansar, J. F., Laeng, B., & Danielsen, A. (2019). Microtiming and Mental Effort: Onset Asynchronies in Musical Rhythm Modulate Pupil Size. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 37(2), 111–133. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.111
Return to Top of Page